Advertisement
Article| Volume 32, ISSUE 1, P113-120, January 2016

Increasing fertility knowledge and awareness by tailored education: a randomized controlled trial

Published:October 30, 2015DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rbmo.2015.10.008

      Abstract

      Women of reproductive age have insufficient fertility knowledge and awareness. Reproductive lifespan and assisted reproduction are the primary areas in which awareness is lacking. Relatively simple interventions can be used to increase knowledge among university students; however, no intervention has been tested to date in a population with more varied education levels. The aim of this study was to evaluate which intervention most improved fertility knowledge in women attending a fertility centre for oocyte donation. A randomized controlled trial was conducted with three intervention groups: tailored, untailored and control. A questionnaire was administered on the day of the first consultation, and again at the oocyte retrieval. Two hundred and one women were enrolled and completed the pre-test, 109 started the cycle and 90 completed the post-test. The effect of the intervention was measured as the difference between the groups in their score from the pre-test to the post test. Only the tailored group showed a significant increase (+2.5; 95% CI [1.8, 3.3]; P = 0.001). Information relating to a woman's most fertile age and limits for childbearing were the most useful. Tailored oral education, therefore, increases fertility knowledge in young women, particularly in relation to their fertility lifespan.

      Keywords

      Introduction

      Fertility knowledge and awareness of infertility risk factors are modest to low in people of reproductive age in countries with different scores on the Human Development Index (
      • Bunting L.
      • Tsibulsky I.
      • Boivin J.
      Fertility knowledge and beliefs about fertility treatment: findings from the International Fertility Decision-making Study.
      ,
      • Chan C.H.
      • Chan T.H.
      • Peterson B.D.
      • Lampic C.
      • Tam M.Y.
      Intentions and attitudes towards parenthood and fertility awareness among Chinese university students in Hong Kong: a comparison with Western samples.
      ,
      • Hammarberg K.
      • Setter T.
      • Norman R.J.
      • Holden C.A.
      • Michelmore J.
      • Johnson L.
      Knowledge about factors that influence fertility among Australians of reproductive age: a population-based survey.
      ,
      • Maeda E.
      • Sugimori H.
      • Nakamura F.
      • Kobayashi Y.
      • Green J.
      • Suka M.
      • Okamoto M.
      • Boivin J.
      • Saito H.
      A cross sectional study on fertility knowledge in Japan, measured with the Japanese version of Cardiff Fertility Knowledge Scale (CFKS-J).
      ,
      • Peterson B.D.
      • Pirritano M.
      • Tucker L.
      • Lampic C.
      Fertility awareness and parenting attitudes among American male and female undergraduate university students.
      ). What information, however, do people of reproductive age receive about fertility? Sex education programmes in schools have been initiated at varied times in European countries; compulsory sex education classes were introduced into schools in Sweden, Germany and France in 1956, 1970 and 1973, respectively, whereas, in other countries, such as Italy, they are not obligatory. In Spain, a recently enacted law regulates the incorporation of this area of knowledge into the curriculum of high school and medical science students (Law 2/2010), but it is not widely applied. Sex education has traditionally focused on the prevention of unintended pregnancies and sexually transmitted diseases, and it is neither optimal nor systematic. Hence, multiple calls to action can be found have been published (
      • Chapman M.G.
      • Driscoll G.L.
      • Jones B.
      Missed conceptions: the need for education.
      ,
      • Everywoman J.
      Cassandra's prophecy: why we need to tell the women of the future about age-related fertility decline and “delayed” childbearing.
      ,
      • Mazza D.
      • Cannold L.
      • Nagle C.
      • McKay F.
      • Brijnath B.
      Making decisions about fertility–three facts GPs need to communicate to women.
      ,
      • Moos M.K.
      • Dunlop A.L.
      • Jack B.W.
      • Nelson L.
      • Coonrod D.V.
      • Long R.
      • Boggess K.
      • Gardiner P.M.
      Healthier women, healthier reproductive outcomes: recommendations for the routine care of all women of reproductive age.
      ), and further recommendations aimed at improving reproductive health are being introduced in clinical settings (
      • Dunlop A.L.
      • Jack B.
      • Frey K.
      National recommendations for preconception care: the essential role of the family physician.
      ,
      • Johnson K.
      • Posner S.F.
      • Biermann J.
      • Cordero J.F.
      • Atrash H.K.
      • Parker C.S.
      • Boulet S.
      • Curtis M.G.
      • Group C.A.P.C.W.
      Select Panel on Preconception Care
      Recommendations to improve preconception health and health care – United States. A report of the CDC/ATSDR Preconception Care Work Group and the Select Panel on Preconception Care.
      ,
      • Johnson J.A.
      • Tough S.
      Society of Obstetricians and Gynaecologists of Canada
      Delayed child-bearing.
      ,
      • Stern J.
      • Bodin M.
      • Grandahl M.
      • Segeblad B.
      • Axen L.
      • Larsson M.
      • Tyden T.
      Midwives' adoption of the reproductive life plan in contraceptive counselling: a mixed methods study.
      ). These actions, however, are far from universal or compulsory. To date, young men and women are not properly informed about either contraception or preconception (
      • Liu F.
      • Parmerter J.
      • Straughn M.
      Reproductive life planning: a concept analysis.
      ,
      • Moos M.K.
      Preconceptional wellness as a routine objective for women's health care: an integrative strategy.
      ). In particular, young people are seldom, if ever, told about the risk factors for future infertility, such as ageing (
      • Dunson D.B.
      • Baird D.D.
      • Colombo B.
      Increased infertility with age in men and women.
      ). Accurate information about age-related infertility and the limitations of assisted reproductive technniques would be particularly valuable (
      • Dunson D.B.
      • Baird D.D.
      • Colombo B.
      Increased infertility with age in men and women.
      ,
      • Leridon H.
      Can assisted reproduction technology compensate for the natural decline in fertility with age? A model assessment.
      ,
      • Liu K.
      • Case A.
      Advanced reproductive age and fertility.
      ) because possibilities with assisted reproduction techniques are commonly thought to be considerable, if not unlimited (
      • Daniluk J.C.
      • Koert E.
      • Cheung A.
      Childless women's knowledge of fertility and assisted human reproduction: identifying the gaps.
      ,
      • Maheshwari A.
      • Porter M.
      • Shetty A.
      • Bhattacharya S.
      Women's awareness and perceptions of delay in childbearing.
      ,
      • Sabarre K.A.
      • Khan Z.
      • Whitten A.N.
      • Remes O.
      • Phillips K.P.
      A qualitative study of Ottawa university students' awareness, knowledge and perceptions of infertility, infertility risk factors and assisted reproductive technologies (ART).
      ).
      Three randomized controlled trials (RCT) were conducted to evaluate the efficacy of educational interventions aimed at increasing fertility knowledge in young people through different evaluation tests. The distribution of educational online brochures about ertility to a population of psychology students at a university in Australia resulted in a significant increase in knowledge about fertility and the effectiveness of assisted reproduction techniques, and a reported lower desired age for childbearing (
      • Wojcieszek A.M.
      • Thompson R.
      Conceiving of change: a brief intervention increases young adults' knowledge of fertility and the effectiveness of in vitro fertilization.
      ). The authors, however, could not evaluate the long-term effects of the intervention, as the evaluation test and the intervention were conducted on the same day. In another study (
      • Williamson L.E.
      • Lawson K.L.
      • Downe P.J.
      • Pierson R.A.
      Informed reproductive decision-making: the impact of providing fertility information on fertility knowledge and intentions to delay childbearing.
      ), a presentation on fertility was given to a population of female university psychology students in Canada, resulting in a twofold increase in the number of correct answers to the test. Again, the intervention and the test were carried out on the same day (
      • Williamson L.E.
      • Lawson K.L.
      • Downe P.J.
      • Pierson R.A.
      Informed reproductive decision-making: the impact of providing fertility information on fertility knowledge and intentions to delay childbearing.
      ). Finally,
      • Stern J.
      • Larsson M.
      • Kristiansson P.
      • Tyden T.
      Introducing reproductive life plan-based information in contraceptive counselling: an RCT.
      , in the context of a university health centre in Sweden, provided participants with tailored oral and written information about family planning based on their reproductive life plans, and administered the evaluation test two months after the intervention. The tailored oral and written information provided in this study was found to have a positive effect on participants' knowledge of reproduction, even two months after the intervention. Overall, these studies showed the effectiveness of relatively simple interventions, which were also greatly appreciated by most participants. All three tests, however, were carried out among a population of university students, who might be more receptive to the intervention than less educated people.
      Oocyte donors seem to be a suitable target population for fertility information and reproductive health advice. On the one hand, they are women in their twenties, on average, which is an optimal time to provide information about the risks and benefits of delaying childbearing, as perceived by the participants of previous research (
      • Maheshwari A.
      • Porter M.
      • Shetty A.
      • Bhattacharya S.
      Women's awareness and perceptions of delay in childbearing.
      ). On the other hand, oocyte donors are usually childless but are expecting to have a family in the future (
      • Garcia D.
      • Vassena R.
      • Trullenque M.
      • Rodriguez A.
      • Vernaeve V.
      Fertility knowledge and awareness in oocyte donors in Spain.
      ). Finally, different educational levels are represented in this population (primary school, high school and university), such that the effect of an intervention can be assessed without restricting the interpretation to the educated population, which has been the focus of most published research.
      The primary objective of this study was to evaluate the benefit of two educational interventions (tailored and untailored) on fertility knowledge in oocyte donors. Furthermore, the aim was to assess whether these interventions have an effect on the reported ideal age for giving birth to the first and the last child.

      Materials and methods

      Study population

      Inclusion criteria for participation in the study were as follows: women between 18 and 35 years of age, who were candidates for oocyte donation, and who had made their first visit to a large private fertility centre between April and November 2014. The CONSORT diagram, represented in Figure 1, shows the flow of participants from the 214 women selected for the study to the 90 women who finally completed the oocyte donation cycle, and the two questionnaires required to evaluate the effect of the educational interventions. The participation rate was 93.9%.
      Figure thumbnail rbmo1461-fig-0001
      Figure 1CONSORT flowchart for randomized controlled trials.

      Sample size

      A sample size estimation was made (
      • Faul F.
      • Erdfelder E.
      • Lang A.G.
      • Buchner A.
      G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences.
      ) to detect an effect size f of 0.3 (moderate effect; critical F = 3.07) between three groups and setting error I and II at 0.05 and 0.1, respectively, resulting in a total number of 126 individuals (at a 1:1:1 scheme resulting in 32 per group). To enable a 50% response (the proportion of candidate donors that are not accepted in the donation programme after the first consultation for reasons unrelated to the study), the intention was to include 67 women per group. The estimated power of the study for the actual group's size is 89.6% accepting an alpha risk of 0.05.

      Study setting and procedure

      The study has a randomized controlled trial design with three groups, to which participants were randomly allocated on their first visit. The three study groups were: T, ‘tailored information’, U, ‘untailored information’ and C, ‘control group’.
      In the T group, participants were provided with both a written brochure with general information about female fertility and also personalized oral information, depending on the incorrect answers given in the pre-test. This intervention was carried out by healthcare workers with extensive experience interacting with young women. In the U group, only the written brochure was provided. In the C group, neither tailored nor untailored information was provided on the first visit; however, the written brochure was provided at the end of the study after the post-test to provide these women with the opportunity to benefit from the educational intervention, as well. The main investigator (DG) explained to the healthcare workers the procedure for providing the brochures, scoring the questionnaires, and giving feedback to the women.
      The randomization list was computer-generated online using GraphPad QuickCalcs (http://www.graphpad.com/quickcalcs/), and allocation was computerized and password protected.
      The effect of the intervention was measured by a self-report questionnaire administered twice: once on the first visit to the clinic (pre-test) before any intervention was undertaken and again on the day of the oocytes retrieval (post-test). The questionnaires were completed by the participants individually.

      Questionnaire

      The questionnaire consisted of 10 items derived from previously published research (
      • Bunting L.
      • Boivin J.
      Knowledge about infertility risk factors, fertility myths and illusory benefits of healthy habits in young people.
      ,
      • Garcia D.
      • Vassena R.
      • Trullenque M.
      • Rodriguez A.
      • Vernaeve V.
      Fertility knowledge and awareness in oocyte donors in Spain.
      ,
      • Lampic C.
      • Svanberg A.S.
      • Karlstrom P.
      • Tyden T.
      Fertility awareness, intentions concerning childbearing, and attitudes towards parenthood among female and male academics.
      ,
      • Sedlecky K.
      • Rasevic M.
      • Topic V.
      Family planning in Serbia–the perspective of female students from the University of Belgrade.
      ,
      • Virtala A.
      • Vilska S.
      • Huttunen T.
      • Kunttu K.
      Childbearing, the desire to have children, and awareness about the impact of age on female fertility among Finnish university students.
      ), and five additional demographic items (age, country of origin, level of education, number of children), which were not scored. The 10 questions scored covered the following topics: the fertile window within the menstrual cycle, the woman's fertility lifespan, infertility risk factors, when to consult a fertility specialist and oocyte donation indications. The wording of the questionnaire and possible or correct answers are shown in Figure 2 (English translation).
      Figure thumbnail rbmo1461-fig-0002
      Figure 2Questionnaire translated into English. Correct answers in bold.

      Brochure and oral information

      The written brochure covered the same topics as the questionnaire, in the same order, and in accessible language, so that, after being read, one could readily answer all the questions. The readability of the questionnaire and the brochure were calculated using the adaptation of the Flesch degree formula for the Spanish language devised by Fernández Huerta (
      • Blanco Pérez A.
      • Gutiérrez Couto U.
      Legibilidad de las páginas web sobre salud dirigidas a pacientes y lectores de la población general.
      ,
      • Flesch R.
      A new readability yardstick.
      ), reaching a readability grade level of 95.0 (very easy) for the questionnaire and 82.0 (easy) for the brochure, which indicates that they were appropriate for adult readers.

      Statistical analysis

      Changes in the questionnaire total scores (on a 0–10 scale) the pre-test to the post-test were compared among the groups by an omnibus univariate analysis (one-way analysis of variance). When this was significant, paired t-tests were applied to compare the groups.
      The percentage of correct answers for each item at the post-test between the intervention groups was analysed by Pearson's chi-square test. It should be noted that two correct answers were accepted for Q8: fertility starts to slightly decrease “at 25” and “at 30”, which is consistent with the interval 25–29 years used in previous research (
      • Lampic C.
      • Svanberg A.S.
      • Karlstrom P.
      • Tyden T.
      Fertility awareness, intentions concerning childbearing, and attitudes towards parenthood among female and male academics.
      ).
      The effect of educational level on the baseline fertility knowledge was analysed by comparing the pre-test total score between university and non-university educated participants using Student's t-test.
      The Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS version 20.0, IBM Inc., USA) was used for the statistical analyses. P < 0.05 was considered to be statistically significant for all analyses.

      Ethical approval

      Ethical approval for the implementation of this study was obtained from the Ethics Committee of Clínica EUGIN (CEIC EUGIN) on 21 January 2014. All participants in the study agreed to be enrolled and signed the informed consent form before their inclusion. This study is registered at ClinicalTrials.gov under the identifier: NCT02364739.

      Results

      Demographic characteristics

      Participants were 25.3 years of age on average on the day of inclusion. Most (79.6%) were originally from Spain, 31.3% were university educated, and 64.7% were childless. Further information about the demographic characteristics of the total population and each study group is detailed in Table 1. No baseline differences were found between the groups, which reflected that the groups were balanced in these characteristics after randomization. In addition, as Table 2 shows, no significant differences were found in the baseline characteristics or previous knowledge of fertility between the participants who completed the study and those who withdrew after the first consultation.
      Table 1Demographic characteristics at pre-test, overall and by study group.
      Overall (n = 201)Tailored information (n = 67)Untailored information (n = 67)Control (n = 67)
      Age, mean (SD)25.3 (4.7)25.2 (4.9)24.8 (4.4)25.9 (4.7)
      Spanish, n (%)160 (79.6)53 (79.1)50 (74.6)57 (85.1)
      No children, n (%)130 (64.7)43 (64.2.)49 (73.1)38 (56.7)
      University education, n (%)63 (31.3)22 (32.8)25 (37.3)16 (23.9)
      Pre-test total score; mean (SD)3.7 (1.6)3.8 (1.7)3.6 (1.5)3.6 (1.6)
      Ideal age for first child at pre-test, mean (SD)25.8 (3.0)25.8 (3.1)25.9 (2.9)25.6 (3.2)
      Ideal age for last child at pre-test, mean (SD)34.4 (4.0)34.7 (4.0)34.3 (3.7)34.3 (4.2)
      Table 2Demographic characteristics at pre-test, overall and by participant's status.
      P < 0.1 is considered as statistically significant for baseline differences between groups no statistically significant differences were found using Student t-test or Pearson's chi-square test, as appropriate.
      Overall (n = 201)Completed (n = 90)Withdrawn (n = 111)
      Age, mean (SD)24.8 (4.7)25.07 (5.0)24.6 (4.5)
      Spanish, n (%)160 (79.6)71 (78.9)89 (80.2)
      No children, n (%)130 (64.7)56 (62.2)74 (66.7)
      University education, n (%)63 (31.3)25 (27.8)38 (34.2)
      Pre-test total score, mean (SD)3.7 (1.6)3.84 (1.4)3.54 (1.8)
      Ideal age for first child at pre-test, mean (SD)25.8 (3.0)25.4 (3.0)26.1 (3.0)
      Ideal age for last child at pre-test, mean (SD)34.4 (4.0)34.0 (4.1)34.7 (3.8)
      a P < 0.1 is considered as statistically significant for baseline differences between groups no statistically significant differences were found using Student t-test or Pearson's chi-square test, as appropriate.

      Pre-test evaluation

      The pre-test score was 3.7 out of 10 on average. The reported ideal ages for having the first and last child were 25.8 and 34.4 years, respectively. The detailed results for the pre-test overall and by study group are presented in Table 1, showing no differences among the groups before the intervention.
      It is worth noting that 37 (18.4%) of the participants answered that “age does not matter” in regard to getting pregnant (Q2) and 36 (17.9%) thought that a 50-year-old pregnant woman was more likely to have become pregnant naturally than if she had undergone assisted reproductive techniques (Q10).
      The analysis of the pre-test scores by level of education showed that university-educated participants had higher fertility knowledge scores than non-university educated participants (4.0 points, 95% confidence interval (CI) [3.6, 4.5] versus 3.5 points, 95% CI [3.3, 3.8], P = 0.043). Just one question, however (Q1, fertile window within menstrual cycle), was answered correctly by significantly more university-educated participants (54.0% versus 31.2%; P = 0.006). Age, being of Spanish origin and having children did not have an effect on the pre-test scores.

      Post-test results

      The intervention increased the average post-test score in all groups compared with the pre-test (T = +2.5, 95% CI [1.8, 3.3]; U = +1.3, 95% CI [0.5, 2.1]; C = +0.42, 95% CI [–0.2, 1.1]), with a marked trend (ptrend < 0.001) towards increasing change across the control, untailored and tailored groups. In the paired comparisons, however, the increase was significant in the T versus C (P = 0.001) and in the T versus U (P = 0.022), but not in the U versus C. A reduction was observed in the ideal age for having the last child after the intervention in the T group, although it did not reach statistical significance (–2.1 years, 95% CI [–3.6, −0.5]). The mean differences in the changes in total score and ideal ages for having the first and the last child are detailed in Table 3.
      Table 3Change in questionnaire total score change and change in ideal age at first and last child, overall and by study group.
      P ≤ 0.05 has been set as statistically significant. NS = not statistically significant.
      Overall (n = 90)Tailored information (n = 31)Untailored information (n = 33)Control (n = 26)P
      Oneway ANOVA.
      ptrend
      Oneway ANOVA with orthogonal linear contrast.
      Change in total score, mean (SD)1.47 (2.2)2.50 (2.1)1.30 (2.3)0.42 (1.7)0.001
      Paired t-test: T vs C: P < 0.001, U vs C: NS, T vs U: P = 0.022.
      <0.001
      Change in ideal age at first pregnancy, mean (SD)−0.44 (2.8)–0.84 (2.7)0.06 (2.4)–0.77 (3.3)NSNS
      Change in ideal age at last pregnancy, mean (SD)–0.99 (4.1)–2.06 (4.3)–0.50 (3.6)–0.31 (4.3)NSNS
      a P ≤ 0.05 has been set as statistically significant. NS = not statistically significant.
      b Oneway ANOVA.
      c Oneway ANOVA with orthogonal linear contrast.
      d Paired t-test: T vs C: P < 0.001, U vs C: NS, T vs U: P = 0.022.
      Overall, the question best answered at the post-test in all the three groups was Q10 (pregnancy at 50 years old) (86.4%), followed by Q7 (infertility risk factors) (64.8%). The T intervention resulted in a significantly greater percentage of correct answers for three items compared with the C group, all of which were related to fertility decreasing with age: Q2 (best age for childbearing; P = 0.042), Q4 (marked fertility decrease; P = 0.023), and Q6 (>35 years old as strong infertility risk; P < 0.001). The U intervention resulted in a greater percentage of correct answers that were higher than the C intervention, although this was not statistically significant. The percentages of correct answers for each question at post-test, overall and by study group are presented in Table 4.
      Table 4Percentage of correct answers to each question after intervention (post-test), overall and by study groups.
      P ≤ 0.05 has been set as statistically significant.
      Overall (n = 90)Tailored information (n = 31)Untailored information (n = 33)Control (n = 26)P
      Pearson's chi-square test.
      Q154.551.662.548.0NS
      Q260.777.456.346.20.045
      Q343.258.139.429.2NS
      Q433.348.433.315.40.031
      Q557.366.754.550.0NS
      Q650.080.037.529.2<0.001
      Q764.877.456.360.0NS
      Q850.048.443.760.0NS
      Q942.543.334.452.0NS
      Q1086.496.884.476.0NS
      NS = not statistically significant.
      a P ≤ 0.05 has been set as statistically significant.
      b Pearson's chi-square test.

      Discussion

      The primary finding of this study is that tailored oral education significantly increases fertility knowledge in oocyte donors, which is true regardless of their level of education. The increase in fertility knowledge, however, only had a modest effect reducing the reported ideal age for childbearing.
      Fertility knowledge at the pre-test was low and increased by the post-test in the three study groups, but this increase was statistically significant only in the T group. We think that the “tailored oral education” was indeed a factor in this outcome for two reasons: one is the randomized nature of our trial, where study groups were successfully balanced at baseline. Another reason is that the information provided to participants in the two intervention groups (U and T) was the same, but the form of delivery was different. In the T group, the information was provided orally and was individualized –drawing the woman's attention to the incorrect answers that she gave, whereas in the U group, the information was given in a standard, non-customized written form. Therefore, we conclude that the observed increase was due to the manner in which the information was given. It is true that we observed a trend toward higher fertility knowledge across all groups (from no information to tailored information); the slight improvement in the no information group might be due to either the effect of how the questionnaire was completed, which might have aroused curiosity on the topic among the women, or to the fact that, during the initial gynaecological evaluation and the follicular control visits at the clinic, women might have acquired information about fertility while on the premises. The improvement observed in the U group, which was lower than in the T group, suggests that written brochures do not elicit sufficient interest if they are not accompanied by individualized information, which is more likely to be retained by the participants. Perhaps online brochures would be more attractive to young people, who could browse the aspects they are most interested in, which might result in significantly increased fertility knowledge (
      • Daniluk J.C.
      • Koert E.
      Fertility awareness online: the efficacy of a fertility education website in increasing knowledge and changing fertility beliefs.
      ,
      • Wantland D.J.
      • Portillo C.J.
      • Holzemer W.L.
      • Slaughter R.
      • McGhee E.M.
      The effectiveness of Web-based vs. non-Web-based interventions: a meta-analysis of behavioral change outcomes.
      ,
      • Wojcieszek A.M.
      • Thompson R.
      Conceiving of change: a brief intervention increases young adults' knowledge of fertility and the effectiveness of in vitro fertilization.
      ). On the whole, the increase in fertility knowledge after the educational interventions observed in previous studies (
      • Daniluk J.C.
      • Koert E.
      Fertility awareness online: the efficacy of a fertility education website in increasing knowledge and changing fertility beliefs.
      ,
      • Stern J.
      • Larsson M.
      • Kristiansson P.
      • Tyden T.
      Introducing reproductive life plan-based information in contraceptive counselling: an RCT.
      ,
      • Wojcieszek A.M.
      • Thompson R.
      Conceiving of change: a brief intervention increases young adults' knowledge of fertility and the effectiveness of in vitro fertilization.
      ) could be explained by the high educational level of the participants. In our study, we have found that having a university education had a significant effect on basal fertility knowledge, but this effect was not found for the difference in scores after the intervention. Therefore, our data do not support the hypothesis that there is a stronger benefit of the intervention in more educated participants, but we recognize that the study was not designed to detect this difference.
      The question that was answered best in all three groups on the pre-test and at post-test was related to the treatment option of oocyte donation for age-related infertility. It was expected that this question would be correctly answered among these women who were candidates for oocyte donation. Similarly, oral contraceptives were correctly identified not to be a risk factor for infertility by more than one-half of the participants at the pre-test, as well as at the post-test, possibly owing to the extensive information about contraceptive methods currently available and to their frequent use among women of this age range (35% of oral contraceptives users in our previous research) (
      • Garcia D.
      • Vassena R.
      • Trullenque M.
      • Rodriguez A.
      • Vernaeve V.
      Fertility knowledge and awareness in oocyte donors in Spain.
      ). On the other hand, all participants were expected to be more knowledgeable about the fertile window within the menstrual cycle than they in fact were. As previously observed (
      • Garcia D.
      • Vassena R.
      • Trullenque M.
      • Rodriguez A.
      • Vernaeve V.
      Fertility knowledge and awareness in oocyte donors in Spain.
      ), the percentage of correct answers to this question was (the only one) closely related to university education. Questions about age-related fertility limits (best age for childbearing, marked fertility decrease at 35 years of age and being 35 years old and upwards as an infertility risk), which were the linchpin of the intervention, presented the main improvement after intervention T. This finding could be observed for two reasons: first, the content of the information given was especially focused on the effect of age on fertility (five out of 10 questions), and therefore might have been better retained overall. Second, all people are affected by age, but non-smokers, for instance, might not pay attention to information about the effect of smoking on fertility.
      We observed inconsistencies in the answers of certain participants. First, a number of participants answered that “age does not matter” for getting pregnant, whereas they recognized a decrease in fertility after the age of 40 years. These answers could be interpreted as “age would not matter while a woman menstruates”, as mentioned by one participant in the study of Littleton with teenage girls (
      • Littleton F.K.
      How teen girls think about fertility and the reproductive lifespan. Possible implications for curriculum reform and public health policy.
      ). Therefore, the relationship between menopause, age, and fertility should not be forgotten in further interventions. Second, we observed a misconception of when fertility “slightly” decreases (before the age of 30 years) and when it “markedly” decreases (from the age of 35 years) because certain participants answered that there was a marked decrease in fertility at a younger age, rather than when the slight decrease in fact occurs. This has not been reported in other studies where the question was posed (
      • Lampic C.
      • Svanberg A.S.
      • Karlstrom P.
      • Tyden T.
      Fertility awareness, intentions concerning childbearing, and attitudes towards parenthood among female and male academics.
      ,
      • Peterson B.D.
      • Pirritano M.
      • Tucker L.
      • Lampic C.
      Fertility awareness and parenting attitudes among American male and female undergraduate university students.
      ,
      • Tyden T.
      • Svanberg A.S.
      • Karlstrom P.O.
      • Lihoff L.
      • Lampic C.
      Female university students' attitudes to future motherhood and their understanding about fertility.
      ), although it is agreed that the terms are rather subjective (
      • Skoog Svanberg A.
      • Lampic C.
      • Karlstrom P.O.
      • Tyden T.
      Attitudes toward parenthood and awareness of fertility among postgraduate students in Sweden.
      ). The third misconception is that certain women think that women aged over 35 years have to wait longer than women aged less than 35 years before attending an assisted reproduction specialist, possibly because they understand that getting pregnant takes more time for an older women and therefore are more likely to attend a fertility centre later. When it is necessary to consult a fertility specialist after attempting to conceive unsuccessfully, the woman is likely to need more attention in future interventions because it does not seem clear for participants at either the pre-test or post-test.
      We recognize several limitations to the present study. First, the post-test evaluation could not be obtained from one-half of the participants because they were not enrolled in the donation programme. The lack of differences in baseline characteristics or in the knowledge of fertility between those who completed the study and those who withdrew makes a selection bias unlikely. Second, although our aim was to measure the effect of education on fertility knowledge among young women of different levels of education, and not only in university-educated women, as has been undertaken in previous studies, our sample might not be representative of the general population. Nevertheless, what our sample does offer is a perspective on young women's fertility knowledge beyond the group of university students, which has been the sole focus of much of the published research.
      In conclusion, this study demonstrates that educational interventions are more effective when they are interesting for the intended audience and tailored to individuals. First, it is necessary to identify which aspects of fertility are matters of concern in the target population. Second, a degree of individualization could be achieved by measuring individual knowledge (through the calculation of a total score or the identification of knowledge gaps), and the translation of the received information into individual risk (how the information can be applied to the individual). Tailored oral information accompanying a written brochure provided in a healthcare centre has been demonstrated to be useful for increasing fertility knowledge in women of reproductive age. Interventions in settings other than at fertility clinics, e.g. interventions aimed at young men, how to improve sex education at school, the promotion of reproductive health campaigns, will be needed to bring about a decrease in childbearing age in the current and future generations or at least to help young women and men have realistic expectations regarding their reproductive life plans.

      Acknowledgements

      The authors wish to thank the oocyte donation team of Clinica EUGIN for recruiting participants and providing information, and Francesc Figueras for statistical support.

      References

        • Blanco Pérez A.
        • Gutiérrez Couto U.
        Legibilidad de las páginas web sobre salud dirigidas a pacientes y lectores de la población general.
        Rev. Esp. Salud Pública. 2002; 76: 321-331
        • Bunting L.
        • Boivin J.
        Knowledge about infertility risk factors, fertility myths and illusory benefits of healthy habits in young people.
        Hum. Reprod. 2008; 23: 1858-1864
        • Bunting L.
        • Tsibulsky I.
        • Boivin J.
        Fertility knowledge and beliefs about fertility treatment: findings from the International Fertility Decision-making Study.
        Hum. Reprod. 2013; 28: 385-397
        • Chan C.H.
        • Chan T.H.
        • Peterson B.D.
        • Lampic C.
        • Tam M.Y.
        Intentions and attitudes towards parenthood and fertility awareness among Chinese university students in Hong Kong: a comparison with Western samples.
        Hum. Reprod. 2015; 30: 364-372
        • Chapman M.G.
        • Driscoll G.L.
        • Jones B.
        Missed conceptions: the need for education.
        Med. J. Aust. 2006; 184: 361-362
        • Daniluk J.C.
        • Koert E.
        Fertility awareness online: the efficacy of a fertility education website in increasing knowledge and changing fertility beliefs.
        Hum. Reprod. 2015; 30: 353-363
        • Daniluk J.C.
        • Koert E.
        • Cheung A.
        Childless women's knowledge of fertility and assisted human reproduction: identifying the gaps.
        Fertil. Steril. 2012; 97: 420-426
        • Dunlop A.L.
        • Jack B.
        • Frey K.
        National recommendations for preconception care: the essential role of the family physician.
        J. Am. Board Fam. Med. 2007; 20: 81-84
        • Dunson D.B.
        • Baird D.D.
        • Colombo B.
        Increased infertility with age in men and women.
        Obstet. Gynecol. 2004; 103: 51-56
        • Everywoman J.
        Cassandra's prophecy: why we need to tell the women of the future about age-related fertility decline and “delayed” childbearing.
        Reprod. Biomed. Online. 2013; 27: 4-10
        • Faul F.
        • Erdfelder E.
        • Lang A.G.
        • Buchner A.
        G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences.
        Behav. Res. Methods. 2007; 39: 175-191
        • Flesch R.
        A new readability yardstick.
        J. Appl. Psychol. 1948; 32: 221-233
        • Garcia D.
        • Vassena R.
        • Trullenque M.
        • Rodriguez A.
        • Vernaeve V.
        Fertility knowledge and awareness in oocyte donors in Spain.
        Patient Educ. Couns. 2014; 98: 96-101
        • Hammarberg K.
        • Setter T.
        • Norman R.J.
        • Holden C.A.
        • Michelmore J.
        • Johnson L.
        Knowledge about factors that influence fertility among Australians of reproductive age: a population-based survey.
        Fertil. Steril. 2013; 99: 502-507
        • Johnson J.A.
        • Tough S.
        • Society of Obstetricians and Gynaecologists of Canada
        Delayed child-bearing.
        J. Obstet. Gynaecol. Can. 2012; 34: 80-93
        • Johnson K.
        • Posner S.F.
        • Biermann J.
        • Cordero J.F.
        • Atrash H.K.
        • Parker C.S.
        • Boulet S.
        • Curtis M.G.
        • Group C.A.P.C.W.
        • Select Panel on Preconception Care
        Recommendations to improve preconception health and health care – United States. A report of the CDC/ATSDR Preconception Care Work Group and the Select Panel on Preconception Care.
        MMWR Recomm. Rep. 2006; 55: 1-23
        • Lampic C.
        • Svanberg A.S.
        • Karlstrom P.
        • Tyden T.
        Fertility awareness, intentions concerning childbearing, and attitudes towards parenthood among female and male academics.
        Hum. Reprod. 2006; 21: 558-564
        • Leridon H.
        Can assisted reproduction technology compensate for the natural decline in fertility with age? A model assessment.
        Hum. Reprod. 2004; 19: 1548-1553
        • Littleton F.K.
        How teen girls think about fertility and the reproductive lifespan. Possible implications for curriculum reform and public health policy.
        Hum. Fertil. (Camb.). 2014; 17: 180-187
        • Liu F.
        • Parmerter J.
        • Straughn M.
        Reproductive life planning: a concept analysis.
        Nurs. Forum. 2015; https://doi.org/10.1111/nuf.12122
        • Liu K.
        • Case A.
        Advanced reproductive age and fertility.
        J. Obstet. Gynaecol. Can. 2011; 33: 1165-1175
        • Maeda E.
        • Sugimori H.
        • Nakamura F.
        • Kobayashi Y.
        • Green J.
        • Suka M.
        • Okamoto M.
        • Boivin J.
        • Saito H.
        A cross sectional study on fertility knowledge in Japan, measured with the Japanese version of Cardiff Fertility Knowledge Scale (CFKS-J).
        Reprod. Health. 2015; 12: 10
        • Maheshwari A.
        • Porter M.
        • Shetty A.
        • Bhattacharya S.
        Women's awareness and perceptions of delay in childbearing.
        Fertil. Steril. 2008; 90: 1036-1042
        • Mazza D.
        • Cannold L.
        • Nagle C.
        • McKay F.
        • Brijnath B.
        Making decisions about fertility–three facts GPs need to communicate to women.
        Aust. Fam. Physician. 2012; 41: 343-346
        • Moos M.K.
        Preconceptional wellness as a routine objective for women's health care: an integrative strategy.
        J. Obstet. Gynecol. Neonatal Nurs. 2003; 32: 550-556
        • Moos M.K.
        • Dunlop A.L.
        • Jack B.W.
        • Nelson L.
        • Coonrod D.V.
        • Long R.
        • Boggess K.
        • Gardiner P.M.
        Healthier women, healthier reproductive outcomes: recommendations for the routine care of all women of reproductive age.
        Am. J. Obstet. Gynecol. 2008; 199: S280-S289
        • Peterson B.D.
        • Pirritano M.
        • Tucker L.
        • Lampic C.
        Fertility awareness and parenting attitudes among American male and female undergraduate university students.
        Hum. Reprod. 2012; 27: 1375-1382
        • Sabarre K.A.
        • Khan Z.
        • Whitten A.N.
        • Remes O.
        • Phillips K.P.
        A qualitative study of Ottawa university students' awareness, knowledge and perceptions of infertility, infertility risk factors and assisted reproductive technologies (ART).
        Reprod. Health. 2013; 10: 41
        • Sedlecky K.
        • Rasevic M.
        • Topic V.
        Family planning in Serbia–the perspective of female students from the University of Belgrade.
        Eur. J. Contracept. Reprod. Health Care. 2011; 16: 469-479
        • Skoog Svanberg A.
        • Lampic C.
        • Karlstrom P.O.
        • Tyden T.
        Attitudes toward parenthood and awareness of fertility among postgraduate students in Sweden.
        Gend. Med. 2006; 3: 187-195
        • Stern J.
        • Larsson M.
        • Kristiansson P.
        • Tyden T.
        Introducing reproductive life plan-based information in contraceptive counselling: an RCT.
        Hum. Reprod. 2013; 28: 2450-2461
        • Stern J.
        • Bodin M.
        • Grandahl M.
        • Segeblad B.
        • Axen L.
        • Larsson M.
        • Tyden T.
        Midwives' adoption of the reproductive life plan in contraceptive counselling: a mixed methods study.
        Hum. Reprod. 2015; 30: 1146-1155
        • Tyden T.
        • Svanberg A.S.
        • Karlstrom P.O.
        • Lihoff L.
        • Lampic C.
        Female university students' attitudes to future motherhood and their understanding about fertility.
        Eur. J. Contracept. Reprod. Health Care. 2006; 11: 181-189
        • Virtala A.
        • Vilska S.
        • Huttunen T.
        • Kunttu K.
        Childbearing, the desire to have children, and awareness about the impact of age on female fertility among Finnish university students.
        Eur. J. Contracept. Reprod. Health Care. 2011; 16: 108-115
        • Wantland D.J.
        • Portillo C.J.
        • Holzemer W.L.
        • Slaughter R.
        • McGhee E.M.
        The effectiveness of Web-based vs. non-Web-based interventions: a meta-analysis of behavioral change outcomes.
        J. Med. Internet Res. 2004; 6: e40
        • Williamson L.E.
        • Lawson K.L.
        • Downe P.J.
        • Pierson R.A.
        Informed reproductive decision-making: the impact of providing fertility information on fertility knowledge and intentions to delay childbearing.
        J. Obstet. Gynaecol. Can. 2014; 36: 400-405
        • Wojcieszek A.M.
        • Thompson R.
        Conceiving of change: a brief intervention increases young adults' knowledge of fertility and the effectiveness of in vitro fertilization.
        Fertil. Steril. 2013; 100: 523-529

      Biography

      Désirée García is a PhD candidate at Barcelona University. She obtained her Bachelor of Science degree in Pharmacy in 2005 and her Master's of Research Methodology and Statistics for Health Sciences in 2015. In 2009, she joined Fundació Privada EUGIN in Barcelona, where her current research focuses on reproductive medicine. Her main interest is evaluating women's fertility knowledge and the path to increase it. She is developing investigation lines in the context of primary fertility care.